J Neurol Res
Journal of Neurology Research, ISSN 1923-2845 print, 1923-2853 online, Open Access
Article copyright, the authors; Journal compilation copyright, J Neurol Res and Elmer Press Inc
Journal website http://www.neurores.org

Original Article

Volume 1, Number 1, April 2011, pages 11-15


Atrophy in the Medial Temporal Lobe is Specifically Associated With Encoding and Storage of Verbal Information in MCI and Alzheimer Patients

Mebeline E. Boona, Rene J.F. Melisa, Marcel G.M. Olde Rikkerta, Roy P.C. Kesselsa, b, c, d

aDepartment of Geriatrics and Alzheimer Center Nijmegen, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center, The Netherlands
bDepartment of Medical Psychology, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center, The Netherlands
cDonders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands
dCorresponding author: R.P.C. Kessels, Department of Medical Psychology, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center, Internal post 925, Postbus 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands

Manuscript accepted for publication April 22, 2011
Short title: MTA in MCI and Alzheimer Patients
doi: https://doi.org/10.4021/jnr18w

Abstract▴Top 

Background: Alzheimer’s dementia (AD) is characterized by a progressive decline in the encoding and storage of episodic memory. Word list learning tests can characterize different aspects of episodic memory. Medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) seems to be an important anatomical feature of AD and its prodromal stage, mild cognitive impairment (MCI). The aim of this study is to define the relationships of the functional memory processes and MTA in patients with AD and MCI, while correcting for confounding factors.

Methods: MTA was evaluated with a visual assessment scale using the MRIs of 53 patients diagnosed with AD or MCI and 19 controls. Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test was used to assess the different aspects of memory processing, i.e., encoding, storage and retrieval. Multiple regression analysis was used to investigate the association of MTA with these different memory processes. In addition to general factors such as age, education and sex, white matter lesions, cortical and subcortical atrophy were evaluated for being confounders.

Results: MTA was significantly associated with encoding (corrected beta = -0.45, sd = 0.11, P < 0.01), and storage (beta = -0.34, sd = 0.12, P < 0.01), but not with retrieval (beta = -0.18, sd = 0.12, P = 0.44).

Conclusions: We can conclude that atrophy of the medial temporal lobe is associated with a decline in encoding and storage of verbal information in MCI and AD.

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease; Mild Cognitive Impairment; Episodic memory; Medial temporal lobe; Magnetic resonance imaging; Response bias

Introduction▴Top 

Alzheimer’s dementia (AD) is a syndrome that is characterized by a progressive decline in multiple cognitive functions. Memory impairment is the key characteristic that is already present in preclinical stages of the disease, often referred to as amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [1]. Episodic memory, that is the ability to acquire explicit information, is affected most. In contrast, memory of events that occurred earlier in life remains intact for a long time. This reflects deficits in the processes encoding and storage, in contrast to retrieval which is often preserved until late in the disease [2].

There is abundant evidence that the delayed recall score of word-list learning tasks, such as Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), is the best neuropsychological parameter for discriminating AD patients from healthy controls [2]. This test can also be used to characterize the different functional memory processes. Moreover, the tendency to favor “yes” or “no” responses during a recognition task, the response bias, can be quantified [3]. Although patients with AD may have a high rate of false alarms, reflecting a liberal response bias, the value of response bias in the differential diagnosis is not clear and data in MCI are lacking altogether.

With the growing availability of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in clinical practice, neuroanatomical characteristics of AD have also been investigated and are increasingly used to support the diagnosis in AD. Atrophy of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) is among the most distinctive neuroanatomical signs. Studies that have investigated the diagnostic value of volumetric and visual assessments of medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) have indeed proven that these measurements can discern AD patients from normal controls [4]. Next to MTA, general atrophy and white-matter pathology are often present in AD patients.

Although the notion that the MTL is important for memory formation is well established [5], it is less clear whether the specific episodic memory impairment in AD and MCI is directly linked with MTA. Previous studies have studied this relationship and found an association between MTA and memory decline [6, 7]. However, the influence of covariates such as education level or white-matter pathology has not been taken into account. Furthermore, in these studies memory was considered a unitary function and separate analyses of the different functional processes have not been performed. Next to descriptive analysis of the different MRI and RAVLT variables, including results on response bias, we investigated the neuropsychological correlates of MTA in both MCI and AD patients while correcting for white-matter lesions (WMLs), as well as cortical and subcortical atrophy. Our hypothesis is that storage and encoding specifically rely on the MTL and therefore are inversely associated with MTA in both AD and MCI patients. Retrieval, on the other hand, will be less associated with the MTA score.

Materials and Methods▴Top 

Data of 53 patients were collected from a database of the memory clinic of Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center for this cross-sectional retrospective study. For patients with the diagnosis of possible or probable AD (NINCDS-ARDRA [8]) or MCI [1], an available MRI scan and an administered RAVLT were included. In addition, data from 19 healthy volunteers who responded to a newspaper advertisement were included. The diagnosis for each patient was made by multidisciplinary consensus. MR imaging examinations were performed using a 1.5 Tesla system (Siemens) and a routine imaging protocol for dementia. All MRI ratings were performed by one rater (MB), who was blind to clinical data. 3D high-resolution T1 weighted coronal slices were used to score the MTA. T2 weighted, fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images were used to assess WMLs, cortical and subcortical atrophy. Linear measurements of subcortical atrophy included the bifrontal ratio, bicaudate ratio and the bioccipital ratio [9]. A measure of cortical atrophy was obtained by dividing the width of the Sylvian fissure by the maximal brain width [9]. White-matter abnormalities were scored according to the scale proposed by Wahlund [10]. The visual rating scale proposed by Scheltens was used to assess MTA [11]. This scale distinguishes between 4 stages of atrophy, which are discerned based on the width of the choroid fissure, width of the temporal horn and height of the hippocampus. All participants performed a brief neuropsychological screening including the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the RAVLT [12]. During the RAVLT, participants had to memorize a list of 15 words, which is presented and tested repeatedly over five learning trials. The participants were asked to recall the words after a delay of 20 minutes (delayed recall), followed by a “yes/no” recognition task of the words from the list intermixed with 15 distracter words. A discrimination index was calculated from the recognition task using the signal detection theory [3]. The C-index was selected to assess the response bias [3].

Linear regression models were used to evaluate the association between the MTA score (left and right averaged) and the memory processes. Memory encoding was quantified with the recall score after the first trial. The storage process was defined as the ability to retain the information stored after the fifth trial over the 20 minutes delay, and was quantified with the discrimination index from the recognition score corrected for the words learned after the fifth trial. The delayed recall task is more dependent on the retrieval process as the recognition task; therefore the retrieval process was quantified with the difference between these two tasks. Age, sex and education level were entered in the model as independent variables. WMLs, cortical and subcortical atrophy were investigated for being confounders, and were entered in the models if they were.

Results▴Top 

Table 1 lists the results on the RAVLT and the MR findings for the controls, AD and MCI patients. Uncorrected regression analysis showed a significant association between MTA-scores and general cognitive function, as measured with the MMSE (beta = -0.68, sd = 0.08, P < 0.01). After correction for storage and encoding processes the association was considerably diminished (beta = -0.26, sd = 0.10, P = 0.01). With inclusion of the remaining confounders (age, sex, education, subcortical atrophy and WMLs of the left hemisphere) the association remained roughly the same (beta = -0.24, sd = 0.11, P = 0.04). The raw association between the storage process and MTA-scores was significant (beta = -0.26, sd = 0.10, P = 0.01). After correcting for confounders (age, sex, education, cortical and subcortical atrophy, WMLs in the left hemisphere), the association increased and remained significant (beta = -0.34, sd = 0.12, P < 0.01). The raw regression analysis of the encoding process and MTA-scores showed a significant association (beta = -0.54, sd = 0.09, P < 0.01). With the addition of the confounding factors (cortical and subcortical atrophy and WMLs in the pons) to the equation the association remained significant (beta = -0.45, sd = 0.11, P < 0.01). Univariate association of the retrieval process and MTA-scores was small and not significant and remains that way (beta = -0.18, sd = 0.12, P = 0.44) when corrected for confounders (age, sex, education level, WMLs in the occipital lobe, cortical atrophy).

Table 1.
Click to view
Table 1. Results (Means ± SD or Percentage) on the Word-List Memory Task and MR Findings for the Controls, MCI Patients and AD Patients
 
Discussion▴Top 

Although there is abundant evidence that the MTL is important for episodic memory formation [5], to date it was unclear whether the specific episodic memory impairment in patients with AD pathology is directly associated with MTA. This study showed that, after taking covariates into account, the decline in memory encoding and storage, which is the neuropsychological hallmark of AD pathology, was most strongly linked to MTA in AD and MCI patients. Initially, the MMSE was also strongly associated with MTA, but the association largely disappeared by correcting for the storage and encoding processes. Secondary, we found a trend towards a more liberal response bias in AD patients as compared to controls and MCI patients, which is in accordance with previous studies [14], but no significant between-group differences were found because of great within-group differences.

The present study extends previous research in that we included both MCI and AD patients and corrected for several MRI parameters, which helped expose the specific association of MTA with encoding and storage of verbal information. Especially cortical atrophy proved to be a confounding factor in the investigation of the association of MTA and cognitive functions. This is understandable as cortical atrophy is associated with a wide range of cognitive deficits. Future research should also correct for these MRI features. In contrast with previous results that have used volumetric measurements of the MTL, we used a visual rating scale. Comparative studies have found good correlations between these two techniques, and even suggest that the visual rating scale is more accurate for the diagnosis of AD [15]. Moreover, it is a scale that is widely used in clinical practice.

We can conclude from this study that specifically encoding and storage impairments in AD and MCI patients were directly associated with MTA. This further elucidates the relationship between the neuro-anatomical and neuropsychological features of Alzheimer disease and offers extra legitimacy for MTA measurements as a diagnostic tool for AD pathology. In this study we confirmed that AD patients tend to have a liberal response bias. To further broaden our understanding of the disease mechanisms, it would be interesting to investigate the neuro-anatomical correlates of this liberal response bias.

Acknowledgments

RPCK was funded by a VIDI innovational grant from the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO, no. 452-08-005).


References▴Top 
  1. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56(3):303-308.
    pubmed doi
  2. Becker J, Overman A. The memory deficit in Alzheimer’s Disease. In: Baddeley A, Kopelman M, Wilson B, eds. The handbook of memory disorders. John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 2002;569-589.
  3. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen 1988;117(1):34-50.
    pubmed doi
  4. Laakso MP, Hallikainen M, Hanninen T, Partanen K, Soininen H. Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: MRI of the hippocampus vs delayed recall. Neuropsychologia 2000;38(5):579-584.
    pubmed doi
  5. Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science 1991;253(5026):1380-1386.
    pubmed doi
  6. de Toledo-Morrell L, Dickerson B, Sullivan MP, Spanovic C, Wilson R, Bennett DA. Hemispheric differences in hippocampal volume predict verbal and spatial memory performance in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Hippocampus 2000;10(2):136-142.
    pubmed doi
  7. Petersen RC, Jack CR, Jr., Xu YC, Waring SC, O'Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Memory and MRI-based hippocampal volumes in aging and AD. Neurology 2000;54(3):581-587.
    pubmed
  8. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 1984;34(7):939-944.
    pubmed
  9. Gomori JM, Steiner I, Melamed E, Cooper G. The assessment of changes in brain volume using combined linear measurements. A CT-scan study. Neuroradiology 1984;26(1):21-24.
    pubmed doi
  10. Wahlund LO, Barkhof F, Fazekas F, Bronge L, Augustin M, Sjogren M, Wallin A, et al. A new rating scale for age-related white matter changes applicable to MRI and CT. Stroke 2001;32(6):1318-1322.
    pubmed
  11. Scheltens P, Fox N, Barkhof F, De Carli C. Structural magnetic resonance imaging in the practical assessment of dementia: beyond exclusion. Lancet Neurol 2002;1(1):13-21.
    pubmed doi
  12. Rey A. L’examen clinicique in psychology. Paris: Press universitaire de France, 1964.
  13. Verhage F. Intelligentie en leeftijd: onderzoek bij Nederlanders van twaalf tot zevenenzeventig jaar [Intelligence and age: Study with Dutch people from age 12 to 77]. Assen: Van Gorcum, 1964.
  14. Kramer JH, Rosen HJ, Du AT, Schuff N, Hollnagel C, Weiner MW, Miller BL, et al. Dissociations in hippocampal and frontal contributions to episodic memory performance. Neuropsychology 2005;19(6):799-805.
    pubmed doi
  15. Wahlund LO, Julin P, Johansson SE, Scheltens P. Visual rating and volumetry of the medial temporal lobe on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia: a comparative study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;69(5):630-635.
    pubmed doi


This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.


Journal of Neurology Research is published by Elmer Press Inc.

 

Browse  Journals  

 

Journal of Clinical Medicine Research

Journal of Endocrinology and Metabolism

Journal of Clinical Gynecology and Obstetrics

 

World Journal of Oncology

Gastroenterology Research

Journal of Hematology

 

Journal of Medical Cases

Journal of Current Surgery

Clinical Infection and Immunity

 

Cardiology Research

World Journal of Nephrology and Urology

Cellular and Molecular Medicine Research

 

Journal of Neurology Research

International Journal of Clinical Pediatrics

 

 
       
 

Journal of Neurology Research, biannually, ISSN 1923-2845 (print), 1923-2853 (online), published by Elmer Press Inc.                     
The content of this site is intended for health care professionals.
This is an open-access journal distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted
non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Creative Commons Attribution license (Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International CC-BY-NC 4.0)


This journal follows the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) recommendations for manuscripts submitted to biomedical journals,
the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) guidelines, and the Principles of Transparency and Best Practice in Scholarly Publishing.

website: www.neurores.org   editorial contact: editor@neurores.org
Address: 9225 Leslie Street, Suite 201, Richmond Hill, Ontario, L4B 3H6, Canada

© Elmer Press Inc. All Rights Reserved.


Disclaimer: The views and opinions expressed in the published articles are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views or opinions of the editors and Elmer Press Inc. This website is provided for medical research and informational purposes only and does not constitute any medical advice or professional services. The information provided in this journal should not be used for diagnosis and treatment, those seeking medical advice should always consult with a licensed physician.